|
|
|
@ -36,12 +36,12 @@ that subjects typed a bit slower (< 3\%) on \textit{Athena (80 g)} compared to
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Aphrodite (50 g)} and \textit{Hera (35 - 60 g)}. With the differences in
|
|
|
|
|
metrics that are commonly used to measure typing speed more closely related to
|
|
|
|
|
productivity (\gls{WPM}, \gls{AdjWPM}) and the trends that indicate a slight
|
|
|
|
|
difference in operating speed, we can accept our hypothesis that solely a
|
|
|
|
|
difference in actuation force has an impact on typing speed.
|
|
|
|
|
difference in operating speed, we can accept our hypothesis that a difference in
|
|
|
|
|
actuation force, at least indirectly, has an impact on typing speed.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{phga_hyp}[\checkmark]
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{phga_hyp*}[1 $\rightarrow$ \cmark]
|
|
|
|
|
Actuation force has an impact on typing speed (efficiency - speed).
|
|
|
|
|
\end{phga_hyp}
|
|
|
|
|
\end{phga_hyp*}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
% During our telephone interviews 76\% of respondents would have preferred a
|
|
|
|
|
% keyboard with lighter actuation force.
|
|
|
|
@ -65,13 +65,13 @@ fourteen of the twenty-four participants also reported, that \textit{Nyx's}
|
|
|
|
|
light actuation force was the reason for many accidental key presses. It further
|
|
|
|
|
stood out, that as shown in Figure \ref{fig:max_opc_ter}, \textit{Athena} was
|
|
|
|
|
the most accurate keyboard for 58\% of participants and also more accurate than
|
|
|
|
|
keyboard \textit{Own} for eleven of the subjects. This concludes, that a higher
|
|
|
|
|
actuation force has a positive impact on error rate.
|
|
|
|
|
keyboard \textit{Own} for eleven of the subjects. Overall, this concludes, that
|
|
|
|
|
a higher actuation force has a positive impact on error rate.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{phga_hyp}[\checkmark]
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{phga_hyp*}[2 $\rightarrow$ \cmark]
|
|
|
|
|
Higher key actuation force decreases typing errors compared to lower key
|
|
|
|
|
actuation force (efficiency - error rate).
|
|
|
|
|
\end{phga_hyp}
|
|
|
|
|
\end{phga_hyp*}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\textbf{Impact of \gls{TER} on \gls{WPM}}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
@ -81,19 +81,190 @@ this additional relation, we conducted a \gls{LRT} of fixed effects for our
|
|
|
|
|
linear mixed-effects model with two random effects (participant and first/second
|
|
|
|
|
typing test), fixed effect \gls{TER} and response variable \gls{WPM}. The
|
|
|
|
|
results of the \gls{LRT} ($\chi^2(1)$ = 110.44, p = 0.00000000000000022)
|
|
|
|
|
suggest, that the \gls{TER} indeed had an impact on \gls{WPM}. This could have
|
|
|
|
|
been, because every time an error was made, almost all participants decided to
|
|
|
|
|
correct it right away. With a higher error rate, this obviously leads to many
|
|
|
|
|
short interruptions and an increased number of characters that are not taken
|
|
|
|
|
into account when computing the \gls{WPM} metric.
|
|
|
|
|
together with the trends of lower \gls{WPM} with increasing \gls{TER}, visible
|
|
|
|
|
in Figure \ref{fig:reg_ter_wpm}, suggest, that the \gls{TER} indeed had an
|
|
|
|
|
impact on \gls{WPM}. This could have been, because every time an error was made,
|
|
|
|
|
almost all participants decided to correct it right away. With a higher error
|
|
|
|
|
rate, this obviously leads to many short interruptions and an increased number
|
|
|
|
|
of characters that are not taken into account when computing the \gls{WPM}
|
|
|
|
|
metric.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{figure}[H]
|
|
|
|
|
\centering
|
|
|
|
|
\includegraphics[width=1.0\textwidth]{images/reg_ter_wpm}
|
|
|
|
|
\caption{Regression lines for the relation between \gls{TER} and \gls{WPM}.
|
|
|
|
|
The trends indicate a decrease in \gls{WPM} with rising \gls{TER} and
|
|
|
|
|
therefore the existence of a relation between the two metrics}
|
|
|
|
|
\label{fig:reg_ter_wpm}
|
|
|
|
|
\end{figure}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\subsection{Impact of Actuation Force on Satisfaction}
|
|
|
|
|
\label{sec:dis_sati}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
We tried to narrow down the rather broad term ``satisfaction'' to individual
|
|
|
|
|
categories that we, with the information gathered through our literature review
|
|
|
|
|
and telephone interviews, defined as necessary for a positive user experience
|
|
|
|
|
while using a keyboard \cite{giese_sati}. We decided for the following metrics
|
|
|
|
|
to evaluate, whether or not a user experience with a keyboard that features
|
|
|
|
|
lighter actuation forces was more satisfactory:
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{table}[H]
|
|
|
|
|
\centering
|
|
|
|
|
\ra{1.0}
|
|
|
|
|
\small
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{tabular}{l}
|
|
|
|
|
$\rightarrow$ Pragmatic scale from the \glsfirst{UEQ-S} \\
|
|
|
|
|
$\rightarrow$ Score of the additional question \textit{``How satisfied have you been with this keyboard?''}\\
|
|
|
|
|
$\rightarrow$ Results of the \glsfirst{KCQ}\\
|
|
|
|
|
$\rightarrow$ Ranking of the keyboards during semi-structured interview\\
|
|
|
|
|
$\rightarrow$ Ratio of positive and negative feedback for each keyboard during semi-structured interview\\
|
|
|
|
|
\end{tabular}
|
|
|
|
|
\end{table}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\textbf{[\xmark] Pragmatic Scale (\gls{UEQ-S})}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
As described in Section \ref{sec:res_ueqs}, we could not find statistically
|
|
|
|
|
significant differences for any of the test keyboards regarding the pragmatic
|
|
|
|
|
scale of the \gls{UEQ-S}. From visual assessment of the graph shown in Figure
|
|
|
|
|
\ref{fig:ueq_tkbs_res} we could conclude, that there is a slight trend towards a
|
|
|
|
|
more positive rating for keyboards that utilized keyswitches with higher
|
|
|
|
|
actuation forces than \textit{Nyx (35 g)}. This trend in the opposite direction
|
|
|
|
|
of our hypothesized outcome, that lighter actuation force leads to more user
|
|
|
|
|
satisfaction, could be due to the longer familiarization time required for
|
|
|
|
|
keyboards with very light actuation force \cite{gerard_keyswitch}.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\textbf{[\xmark] Additional Question of Satisfaction with Keyboard}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
The results deduced from the additional question \textit{``How satisfied have
|
|
|
|
|
you been with this keyboard?''}, which could be answered on a \glsfirst{VAS}
|
|
|
|
|
from 0 to 100 after both tying tests with a keyboard, suggested that \textit{Nyx
|
|
|
|
|
(35 g)}, the keyboard with the lightest actuation force and also
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Athena (80 g)} the keyboard with the highest actuation force, were rated
|
|
|
|
|
significantly worse than \textit{Aphrodite (50 g)}. Additionally, \textit{Hera
|
|
|
|
|
(35 - 60 g)}, the adjusted keyboard showed a trend towards a significantly
|
|
|
|
|
better rating than \textit{Nyx}. These results indicate, that neither of the
|
|
|
|
|
keyboards with extreme actuation forces were perceived as a overwhelmingly
|
|
|
|
|
pleasant keyboard to use during our typing tests. This is further supported by
|
|
|
|
|
the visualisation of the mean ratings in Figure \ref{fig:res_tkbs_sati} where
|
|
|
|
|
the average ratings for \textit{Aphrodite} and \textit{Hera} were approximately
|
|
|
|
|
10 points higher than those for \textit{Nyx} and \textit{Athena}.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\textbf{[\xmark] Keyboard Comfort Questionnaire (\gls{KCQ})}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
For the \gls{KCQ} we found several statistically significant differences. For
|
|
|
|
|
questions related to effort or fatigue while operating a keyboard,
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Athena (80 g)} received significantly lower ratings than the other test
|
|
|
|
|
keyboards. Additionally to the measured differences in error rates discussed in
|
|
|
|
|
Section \ref{sec:dis_error}, we discovered that participants also perceived the
|
|
|
|
|
accuracy of \textit{Athena (80 g)} and \textit{Aphrodite (50 g)} higher compared
|
|
|
|
|
to \textit{Nyx (35 g)}. Similarly to the results discussed in the last
|
|
|
|
|
paragraph, the scores of the two keyboards with extreme actuation forces,
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Nyx (35 g)} and \textit{Athena (80 g)} fluctuated quite a bit and on
|
|
|
|
|
average those two keyboards scored lower than \textit{Aphrodite (50 g)} or
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Hera (35 - 60 g)} (Figure \ref{fig:kcq_tkbs_res}). Thereby, these
|
|
|
|
|
results do not indicate a clear trend towards enhanced user experience when
|
|
|
|
|
using keyboards with lower actuation forces.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\textbf{[\xmark] Post Experiment Ranking of All Keyboards}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
The ranks in terms of favored test keyboard, provided by all twenty-four
|
|
|
|
|
participants during the post-experiment semi-structured interview, can be
|
|
|
|
|
observed in Figure \ref{fig:tkbs_ranking}. The results further support the
|
|
|
|
|
tendencies towards keyboards with medium actuation forces, that we already
|
|
|
|
|
observed in the last couple paragraphs.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{figure}[H]
|
|
|
|
|
\centering
|
|
|
|
|
\includegraphics[width=0.8\textwidth]{images/tkbs_ranking}
|
|
|
|
|
\caption{Rankings for only the test keyboards, gathered during the
|
|
|
|
|
post-experiment interview. It was possible to rank two or more keyboards the
|
|
|
|
|
same}
|
|
|
|
|
\label{fig:tkbs_ranking}
|
|
|
|
|
\end{figure}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\textbf{[\xmark] Ratio of Positive and Negative Feedback}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Lastly, we analysed all recordings of the post-experiment interviews and
|
|
|
|
|
categorized the feedback given for each keyboard into positive and negative
|
|
|
|
|
responses. We then calculated a ratio of these responses, which can be seen in
|
|
|
|
|
Figure \ref{fig:ratio_interview}, to evaluate preferences towards specific
|
|
|
|
|
keyboards, that could not be expressed by our participants through any other
|
|
|
|
|
supplied method during the experiment. Like all other factors we identified as
|
|
|
|
|
reasonable indicators for satisfaction, these ratios yielded, that neither
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Athena (80 g)} nor \textit{Nyx (35 g)} received more positive than
|
|
|
|
|
negative feedback. It should be noted, that previous research has shown that
|
|
|
|
|
people tend to remember and process bad experiences more thorough than good
|
|
|
|
|
ones, which could be a reason for the increased number of negative feedback for
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Nyx} and \textit{Athena} but would also indicate a worse experience with
|
|
|
|
|
those two keyboards \cite{baumeister_bad}.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{figure}[H]
|
|
|
|
|
\centering
|
|
|
|
|
\includegraphics[width=0.9\textwidth]{images/ratio_interview}
|
|
|
|
|
\caption{The ration of $\frac{Positive Responses}{Negative Responses}$ during
|
|
|
|
|
the semi-structured interview for all test keyboards}
|
|
|
|
|
\label{fig:ratio_interview}
|
|
|
|
|
\end{figure}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\textbf{Conclusion}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Contrary to the responses of our preliminary telephone interview, where 76\% of
|
|
|
|
|
attendees preferred a keyboard with light actuation force, none of the factors
|
|
|
|
|
we defined as relevant for user satisfaction suggests, that keyboards with lower
|
|
|
|
|
actuation force are more satisfactory to use than keyboards with higher
|
|
|
|
|
actuation force. Therefore, we have to fully reject our hypothesis. We can
|
|
|
|
|
conclude thought, that keyboards with actuation forces in between those two
|
|
|
|
|
extremes (35 g / 80 g), are persistently perceived as more pleasant to use and
|
|
|
|
|
that ratings keyboards with extreme actuation forces are highly influenced by
|
|
|
|
|
personal preference, which is indicated by the high fluctuation of almost all
|
|
|
|
|
responses regarding our evaluated factors.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{phga_hyp*}[3 $\rightarrow$ \xmark]
|
|
|
|
|
Keys with lower actuation force are perceived as more satisfactory to type
|
|
|
|
|
with than keys with higher actuation force.
|
|
|
|
|
\end{phga_hyp*}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\subsection{Impact of Actuation Force on Muscle Activity}
|
|
|
|
|
\label{sec:dis_emg}
|
|
|
|
|
In contrast to other studies that suggested, that actuation force has an impact
|
|
|
|
|
on muscle activity, we could not identify significant differences in terms of \%
|
|
|
|
|
of \glsfirst{MVC} for any of our \gls{EMG} measurements. Only a slight trend,
|
|
|
|
|
that \textit{Nyx (35 g)} produced the highest flexor \%\gls{MVC} for only 14\%
|
|
|
|
|
of participants, could be interpreted as anecdotal evidence towards our
|
|
|
|
|
hypothesis, that actuation force has an impact on muscle activity.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{phga_hyp*}[4 $\rightarrow$ \xmark]
|
|
|
|
|
Differences in actuation force influence muscle activity while typing.
|
|
|
|
|
\end{phga_hyp*}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
%\subsection{Impact of an Adjusted Keyboard on Typing Speed, Error Rate and
|
|
|
|
|
% Satisfaction}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\subsection{Impact of an Adjusted Keyboard on Typing Speed, Error Rate and
|
|
|
|
|
Satisfaction}
|
|
|
|
|
\subsection{Implications for the Adjusted Keyboard}
|
|
|
|
|
\label{sec:dis_hera}
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
As discussed in the previous sections, there were no statistically significant
|
|
|
|
|
differences in terms of satisfaction for any of the test keyboards, including
|
|
|
|
|
our adjusted keyboard \textit{Hera}. Still, the rather unconventional design
|
|
|
|
|
choice of non-uniform actuation forces across a keyboard did not negatively
|
|
|
|
|
influence the satisfaction compared to \textit{Aphrodite} which was often
|
|
|
|
|
perceived as equivalent to the participant's own keyboard. In fact,
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Hera} was the keyboard with the most occurrences in the top three, tied
|
|
|
|
|
first place with \textit{Aphrodite} and was never ranked 4th place during the
|
|
|
|
|
post-experiment interview (Figure \ref{fig:tkbs_ranking}). Since \textit{Hera},
|
|
|
|
|
among others, utilized keyswitches with light actuation force (35 g), the
|
|
|
|
|
satisfaction could improve during prolonged usage, because of the longer
|
|
|
|
|
familiarization period required by keyboards with lighter actuation forces
|
|
|
|
|
\cite{gerard_keyswitch}. Interestingly, participant \textit{I3Z4XI7H} who
|
|
|
|
|
reported a currently present disease of the left arm and wrist (Syndrome Sudeck,
|
|
|
|
|
complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS)), ranked Hera 30 points higher than all
|
|
|
|
|
other keyboards. \textit{I3Z4XI7H} also reported in the post-experiment
|
|
|
|
|
interview, that \textit{Hera} was surprisingly pleasant to use and that pain was
|
|
|
|
|
significantly lower than with all other keyboards including
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Own}. However, because of the nearly identical scores to
|
|
|
|
|
\textit{Aphrodite} we have to reject our hypothesis, that an adjusted keyboard
|
|
|
|
|
is more satisfactory to use than standard keyboards.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
\begin{phga_hyp*}[7 $\rightarrow$ \xmark]
|
|
|
|
|
An adjusted keyboard is perceived as more satisfactory to type with compared to standard keyboards.
|
|
|
|
|
\end{phga_hyp*}
|
|
|
|
|